Previous work on this project has shown that value-based decision making requires both the OFC and the amygdala. We have used two kinds of tasks to investigate their mechanisms: (1) standard probabilistic and deterministic 2- and 3-choice visual discrimination learning tasks and (2) tasks that require subjects to make choices associated with rewards of different value (the devaluation task). We have found that both the amygdala and OFC are necessary when subjects need to make choices based on the current value of rewards, but not for choices based simply on the availability of rewards. Our earlier results indicated that when the value of one food was reduced by the subject consuming it to satiety, control subjects showed a reduction in actions associated with the devalued food. In contrast, subjects with either bilateral damage to the amygdala or the OFC failed to show this devaluation effect. In the current review period, we have extended these results by studying the effect of surgical crossed-disconnections of the amygdala and the OFC on the same test. Preliminary results indicate that the functional interaction of the amygdala with the OFC is essential for choices based on current valuations. We recently found that damage limited to cells in the OFC, but sparing fibers, did not affect performance on standard tests of visual discrimination learning and reversal. This led us to reject the influential idea that the primary function of the OFC involves inhibitory control. Although the loss of neurons in OFC per se is not responsible for the impairment, some other part(s) of the frontal cortex is presumably responsible. Accordingly, future studies will seek to identify the specific frontal areas that mediate reward-based choices, as well as the location of white-matter pathways which are disrupted by OFC lesions. Because these findings contradict previous ideas about the function of the OFC, we explored the role of OFC in more demanding reversal learning tasks. We tested subjects on two separate probabilistic tasks. In one test, they saw two stimuli on a touch screen;one option had a 0.75 probability of producing a reward and the other had a 0.25 probability. Once the subjects consistently selected the high-probability option, the stimulus-reward contingencies were reversed. In the second test, subjects saw 3 different stimuli, each with a probability of receiving a reward that fluctuated during a session. On both tasks, subjects with selective lesions of the OFC performed as well as controls. All of our subjects tracked the probability of reward associated with the 3 options and selected the one with highest reward probability. Taken together, these results show that the OFC is not necessary for probabilistic learning and reversal: measures of reward availability as opposed to the current value of a reward. Like these recent findings for the OFC, the amygdala is not necessary for object reversal learning in the standard task. Indeed, amygdala damage leads to a slight facilitation in this form of stimulus-reward learning. Past studies have assessed the performance of subjects when they first experienced reversals as opposed to much later, when performance had reached an asymptote, and they exclusively used deterministic rewards. As such, it is not clear how the amygdala contributes to reversal learning when behavioral strategies are stable and feedback is probabilistic. To address these issues, we examined the role of the amygdala in reversal learning when subjects expected a reversal in the stimulus-reward contingencies. Subjects with bilateral amygdala damage and controls were assessed in two ways. In the first, subjects initially learned to choose between two novel, deterministically rewarded options and the tasks progression was tied to reaching a criterion level of performance after each reversal. In the second, subjects learned to choose between two choice options, but the reversal randomly occurred. Preliminary results show that amygdala damage compromises the ability to reverse stimulus-reward associations by disrupting reinforcement learning when tested in these new ways. Subjects with amygdala damage show heightened sensitivity to negative feedback, compared to controls. Thus, amygdala damage causes erratic choice behavior and heightened sensitivity to negative feedback, which can either enhance or impair decisions depending on the stability of behavioral strategies and knowledge of prior probabilities, in a Bayesian manner. Finally, a considerable body of evidence has suggested a role for the amygdala in processing emotionally salient information in faces. For example, patients with amygdala damage fix their gaze on the eye region of faces to a lesser extent than controls. To evaluate amygdala contributions to social displays of emotion, we had subjects perform an attentional capture task in which they viewed pictures of a socially relevant portion of a subject face (eye, nose, or mouth) or nonsocial pictures. When the stimulus appeared at the subjects fixation point, they had to make a saccade away from it as quickly as possible. For comparison, we also permitted free viewing, during which subjects looked at faces voluntarily. In the free viewing condition, like previously published data, subjects with amygdala lesions spent less time than controls exploring faces, specifically the eye region. In the attentional capture task, we found that all subjects, lesioned and controls, made saccades more slowly when they saw social (vs. nonsocial) stimuli. We also found that saccade latency varied according to the facial feature displayed. Saccade latency was significantly faster in subjects with amygdala damage, compared to controls, especially on trials with a threatening expression formed by the mouth. These data indicate that subjects with amygdala damage have an impairment in attending to certain social cues. Unlike in the free viewing condition, the effect of amygdala damage on the attentional capture task was driven by the mouth region, as opposed to the eyes. These data indicate that the amygdala plays a crucial role in processing and attending to social cues.

Project Start
Project End
Budget Start
Budget End
Support Year
8
Fiscal Year
2014
Total Cost
Indirect Cost
Name
U.S. National Institute of Mental Health
Department
Type
DUNS #
City
State
Country
Zip Code
Murray, Elisabeth A; Rudebeck, Peter H (2018) Specializations for reward-guided decision-making in the primate ventral prefrontal cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci 19:404-417
Taubert, Jessica; Flessert, Molly; Wardle, Susan G et al. (2018) Amygdala lesions eliminate viewing preferences for faces in rhesus monkeys. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 115:8043-8048
Vicario-Feliciano, Raquel; Murray, Elisabeth A; Averbeck, Bruno B (2017) Ventral striatum lesions do not affect reinforcement learning with deterministic outcomes on slow time scales. Behav Neurosci 131:385-91
Fiuzat, Emily C; Rhodes, Sarah E V; Murray, Elisabeth A (2017) The Role of Orbitofrontal-Amygdala Interactions in Updating Action-Outcome Valuations in Macaques. J Neurosci 37:2463-2470
Basile, Benjamin M; Karaskiewicz, Chloe L; Fiuzat, Emily C et al. (2017) MRI Overestimates Excitotoxic Amygdala Lesion Damage in Rhesus Monkeys. Front Integr Neurosci 11:12
Rudebeck, Peter H; Saunders, Richard C; Lundgren, Dawn A et al. (2017) Specialized Representations of Value in the Orbital and Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex: Desirability versus Availability of Outcomes. Neuron 95:1208-1220.e5
Rothenhoefer, Kathryn M; Costa, Vincent D; Bartolo, Ramón et al. (2017) Effects of Ventral Striatum Lesions on Stimulus-Based versus Action-Based Reinforcement Learning. J Neurosci 37:6902-6914
Jang, Anthony I; Costa, Vincent D; Rudebeck, Peter H et al. (2015) The Role of Frontal Cortical and Medial-Temporal Lobe Brain Areas in Learning a Bayesian Prior Belief on Reversals. J Neurosci 35:11751-60
Basile, Benjamin M; Moylan, Emily J; Charles, David P et al. (2015) Two-item same/different discrimination in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Anim Cogn :
Murray, Elisabeth A; Moylan, Emily J; Saleem, Kadharbatcha S et al. (2015) Specialized areas for value updating and goal selection in the primate orbitofrontal cortex. Elife 4:

Showing the most recent 10 out of 20 publications