conceptsC.4.a.(1). Literature review and background Studies in normal subjects have shown that concrete words, compared to abstract ones, have anearlier age of acquisition (Brown, 1957; Centner, 1982), are easier to learn and remember (Paivio, 1991), andare processed faster in lexical decision tasks (James, 1975; Kroll & Merves, 1986). This phenomenon, termedthe 'concreteness effect,' has been addressed at length in several cognitively oriented theoretical accounts(Kieras, 1978; Paivio, 1971, 1991; Plaut & Shallice, 1993; Schwanenflugel, 1991). A strong effect ofconcreteness has also been demonstrated in many brain-damaged patients (see Saffran & Shod, 1999, forreview). For example, aphasics show poorer comprehension and production of abstract words, compared toconcrete ones (e.g., Crutch & Warrington, 2005; Franklin et al., 1995; Goodglass et al., 1969; Goodglass &379Project 5: Memory and Language DAMASIO, ANTONIO R.Wingfield, 1997; Marshall & Newcombe, 1966). This same pattern is typical of patients with deep dyslexia anddeep dysphasia (e.g., Coltheart, 1980; Coltheartet al., 1980; Crutch, 2006; Franklin, 1989; Howard & Franklin,1988; Martin, 1996; Morton & Patterson, 1980). Comparable results have been obtained in regard to high-imageable versus low-imageable words, where high-imageable words tend to have an advantage (e.g.,Franklin et al., 1992; Marshall et al., 1996). Crutch and Warrington (2005) argued that abstract and concreteword meanings are based in representational systems that have qualitatively different properties. Given the potency of the concreteness effect, it is intriguing to find instances in which patientsdemonstrate the opposite pattern, namely, superiority in processing abstract words and concepts (e.g.,Warrington, 1975; Warrington & Shallice, 1984). One of our patients defined duck as 'has fur, four legs, and isabout 4 feet tall'; for health, though, he said, 'the way you are feeling...you should have this to live a longtime.' Other cases of superiority for abstract concepts have been reported (Breedin et al., 1994; Carbonnet etal., 1997; Cipolotti & Warrington, 1995; Marshall et al., 1996; Sirigu et al., 1991; Srinivas et al., 1997). Theavailable literature suggests that a relative superiority for concrete concepts is associated with damage to theperisylvian region of the left hemisphere, whereas superiority for abstract concepts is linked to bilateraldamage to the anterior and inferior temporal lobes. This pattern has received some mixed support fromfunctional imaging studies (Beauregard et al., 1997; Chertkow et al., 1998; Friederici et al., 2000; Noppeney &Price, 2004; Wise et al., 2000). It has been shown that simple imagery of the referents of concrete words(compared to abstract ones) activates structures in the left inferior temporal region (D'Esposito et al., 1997).Binder and colleagues (2005) found that abstract words, relative to concrete words, activated left inferior frontalregions; in general, concrete concepts triggered greater involvement of bilateral association areas, andabstract concepts triggered almost exclusively left hemisphere activation. Whatmough et al. (2004) found adifference between abstract and concrete concepts in the lateralization of fusiform activation (abstract-right;concrete-left); however, both types of concepts activated the left temporal cortex. Fiebach and Friederici(2004) found that abstract words activated a subregion of the left inferior frontal gyrus more strongly thanconcrete words, whereas

Agency
National Institute of Health (NIH)
Institute
National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS)
Type
Research Program Projects (P01)
Project #
2P01NS019632-24A1
Application #
7243732
Study Section
Special Emphasis Panel (ZNS1-SRB-R (21))
Project Start
Project End
Budget Start
2007-06-01
Budget End
2008-05-31
Support Year
24
Fiscal Year
2007
Total Cost
$697,083
Indirect Cost
Name
University of Southern California
Department
Type
DUNS #
072933393
City
Los Angeles
State
CA
Country
United States
Zip Code
90089
Mehta, Sonya; Inoue, Kayo; Rudrauf, David et al. (2016) Segregation of anterior temporal regions critical for retrieving names of unique and non-unique entities reflects underlying long-range connectivity. Cortex 75:1-19
Immordino-Yang, Mary Helen; Yang, Xiao-Fei; Damasio, Hanna (2016) Cultural modes of expressing emotions influence how emotions are experienced. Emotion 16:1033-9
Scherer, Aaron M; Taber-Thomas, Bradley C; Tranel, Daniel (2015) A neuropsychological investigation of decisional certainty. Neuropsychologia 70:206-13
Ceschin, Rafael; Wisnowski, Jessica L; Paquette, Lisa B et al. (2015) Developmental synergy between thalamic structure and interhemispheric connectivity in the visual system of preterm infants. Neuroimage Clin 8:462-72
Philippi, Carissa L; Tranel, Daniel; Duff, Melissa et al. (2015) Damage to the default mode network disrupts autobiographical memory retrieval. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 10:318-26
Guzmán-Vélez, Edmarie; Tranel, Daniel (2015) Does bilingualism contribute to cognitive reserve? Cognitive and neural perspectives. Neuropsychology 29:139-50
Derksen, B J; Duff, M C; Weldon, K et al. (2015) Older adults catch up to younger adults on a learning and memory task that involves collaborative social interaction. Memory 23:612-24
Kumaran, Dharshan; Warren, David E; Tranel, Daniel (2015) Damage to the Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Impairs Learning from Observed Outcomes. Cereb Cortex 25:4504-18
Ceschin, Rafael; Lee, Vince K; Schmithorst, Vince et al. (2015) Regional vulnerability of longitudinal cortical association connectivity: Associated with structural network topology alterations in preterm children with cerebral palsy. Neuroimage Clin 9:322-37
Hannula, Deborah E; Tranel, Daniel; Allen, John S et al. (2015) Memory for items and relationships among items embedded in realistic scenes: disproportionate relational memory impairments in amnesia. Neuropsychology 29:126-38

Showing the most recent 10 out of 353 publications