A typical scene contains many different objects that compete for neural representation due to the limited processing capacity of the visual system. At the neural level, competition among multiple stimuli is evidenced by the mutual suppression of their visually evoked responses and occurs most strongly at the level of the receptive field. The competition among multiple objects can be biased by both bottom-up sensory-driven mechanisms and top-down influences, such as selective attention. Functional brain imaging studies reveal that biasing signals due to selective attention can modulate neural activity in visual cortex not only in the presence but also in the absence of visual stimulation. Although the competition among stimuli for representation is ultimately resolved within visual cortex, the source of top-down biasing signals likely derives from a distributed network of areas in frontal and parietal cortex. This biased competition model of attention suggests that once attentional resources are depleted, no further processing is possible. Yet, existing data suggest that emotional stimuli activate brain regions """"""""automatically,"""""""" largely immune from attentional control. We tested the alternative possibility, namely, that the neural processing of stimuli with emotional content is not automatic and instead requires some degree of attention. Our results revealed that, contrary to the prevailing view, all brain regions responding differentially to emotional faces, including the amygdala, did so only when sufficient attentional resources were available to process the faces. Thus, similar to the processing of other stimulus categories, the processing of facial expression is under top-down control.? ? In the past year, we completed two studies aimed at understanding the role of prefrontal cortex in the control of attention. The first study examined which attentional functions are critically dependent on prefrontal cortex (PFC) by removing PFC unilaterally and transecting the forebrain commissures in two macaques. The monkeys fixated a central cue and discriminated the orientation of a colored target grating presented among colored distracter gratings in either the hemifield affected by the PFC lesion or the normal control hemifield. When the cue was held constant for many trials, task performance in the affected hemifield was nearly normal. However, performance was severely impaired when the cue was switched frequently across trials. The monkeys were unimpaired in a pop-out task with changing targets that did not require top-down attentional control. Thus, the PFC lesion resulted in selective impairment in the monkeys ability to switch top-down control. In the second study, we used fMRI to investigate the neural correlates of top-down control in humans performing tasks identical those used in the monkey experiments. Several fronto-parietal and posterior visual areas showed enhanced activation when attention was switched, which was greater on color cueing (top-down) trials relative to pop-out trials. Taken together, our findings indicate that both frontal and parietal cortices are involved in generating top-down control signals for attentive switching, which may then be fed back to visual processing areas. The PFC in particular plays a critical role in the ability to switch attentional control on the basis of changing task demands.

Agency
National Institute of Health (NIH)
Institute
National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH)
Type
Intramural Research (Z01)
Project #
1Z01MH002035-28
Application #
7735105
Study Section
Project Start
Project End
Budget Start
Budget End
Support Year
28
Fiscal Year
2008
Total Cost
$3,750,016
Indirect Cost
Name
U.S. National Institute of Mental Health
Department
Type
DUNS #
City
State
Country
United States
Zip Code
Tootell, Roger B H; Devaney, Kathryn J; Young, Jeremy C et al. (2008) fMRI mapping of a morphed continuum of 3D shapes within inferior temporal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105:3605-9
Hadj-Bouziane, Fadila; Bell, Andrew H; Knusten, Tamara A et al. (2008) Perception of emotional expressions is independent of face selectivity in monkey inferior temporal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105:5591-6
Heekeren, Hauke R; Marrett, Sean; Ungerleider, Leslie G (2008) The neural systems that mediate human perceptual decision making. Nat Rev Neurosci 9:467-79
Ungerleider, Leslie G; Galkin, Thelma W; Desimone, Robert et al. (2008) Cortical connections of area V4 in the macaque. Cereb Cortex 18:477-99
Mukai, Ikuko; Kim, David; Fukunaga, Masaki et al. (2007) Activations in visual and attention-related areas predict and correlate with the degree of perceptual learning. J Neurosci 27:11401-11
Siman-Tov, Tali; Mendelsohn, Avi; Schonberg, Tom et al. (2007) Bihemispheric leftward bias in a visuospatial attention-related network. J Neurosci 27:11271-8
Rossi, Andrew F; Bichot, Narcisse P; Desimone, Robert et al. (2007) Top down attentional deficits in macaques with lesions of lateral prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 27:11306-14
Pessoa, Luiz; Japee, Shruti; Sturman, David et al. (2006) Target visibility and visual awareness modulate amygdala responses to fearful faces. Cereb Cortex 16:366-75
Heekeren, H R; Marrett, S; Ruff, D A et al. (2006) Involvement of human left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in perceptual decision making is independent of response modality. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103:10023-8
Pessoa, Luiz; Japee, Shruti; Ungerleider, Leslie G (2005) Visual awareness and the detection of fearful faces. Emotion 5:243-7

Showing the most recent 10 out of 41 publications