Although the jury room can be an emotional place, little research has examined the impact of jurors' emotions on the deliberation process. Three studies will investigate how expressing emotion affects the potential for a single juror arguing against the rest of the jury (i.e., a holdout) to influence the majority, particularly holdouts from groups associated with emotion stereotypes (e.g., the "angry Black man" stereotype). Stereotyped minorities are often penalized for expressing emotions. In a holdout juror situation, however, emotion stereotypes might present a unique opportunity for the holdout to violate majority members' expectations, which sometimes enhances the credibility of people who hold a minority opinion. This research will assess people's gender- and race-based emotion stereotypes (Study 1), and will investigate the implications of a holdout juror expressing these stereotypical emotions during jury deliberation (Studies 2 and 3). Participants will engage in what appears to be a computer-mediated discussion about a murder case with five other mock jurors, but in reality is a pre-written fictional deliberation script. These studies will test (a) whether participants are more or less persuaded by holdout jurors who express their opinions with sadness, anger, or no emotion, and (b) if these effects depend on whether the holdout is a man or a woman (for whom sadness is stereotypical and anger is nonstereotypical), or a White versus Black man (for whom anger is stereotypical and sadness is nonstereotypical).

The proposed research will increase understanding about the effect of emotion expression on underrepresented groups? credibility and potential to exert influence in emotionally charged settings (e.g., juries, collaborator meetings, hiring committees, workplace groups). It will also increase understanding of how historically underrepresented jurors can decrease or enhance their credibility through expressing emotion on juries, and ensure that the many efforts to promote diversity in jury selection are not futile.

Project Report

Although juries were originally composed exclusively of white men, U.S. juries now include women and racial minorities. This project was designed to investigate whether previously underrepresented jurors (women, African Americans) have the same opportunity to exert influence over the jury’s decision, compared to White men. Being forced to deliberate at length because one group member disagrees creates hostility. Discussions get heated; negative emotion results. Yet, we know little about how holdout jurors’ emotion expression might influence their ability to exert influence over the majority—particularly if they belong to a historically underrepresented group for whom emotion stereotypes exist (e.g., Black men stereotyped as angry). We predicted that expressing emotion could detract or enhance credibility, depending on the holdout’s gender or race. A set of four experiments investigated how holdout jurors’ emotion expression affects their potential to influence the majority—particularly for stereotyped holdouts. After demonstrating that fear is perceived as stereotypical for women and non-stereotypical for Black men, and anger is non-stereotypical for women and stereotypical for Black men (Study 1), we assessed the effect of holdouts expressing these emotions on their ability to exert influence over the majority during mock deliberation. In three subsequent experiments, participants were told that they would engage in a computer-mediated discussion with five other mock jurors about a murder case, when in reality they were reading a pre-written fictional deliberation script. After viewing trial evidence, participants reported their initial verdict preference, verdict confidence and wrote arguments to contribute to what they believed to be a real computer-mediated deliberation with five other participants. All participants saw the same false pre-determined feedback, however, in which four pre-programmed confederate jurors agreed with the participant and one confederate holdout argued for the opposite verdict. After each round of comments, participants reported their verdict confidence. We manipulated whether the confederate holdout (a) expressed opinions with no emotion, anger, or fear, and (b) was a man or woman (Study 2), or a White or Black man (Studies 3-4). When holdouts expressed no emotion or fear, the participants’ confidence in their original verdict was unaffected by the holdouts’ arguments. The effect of expressing anger on the holdout’s ability to influence the majority depended on the holdout’s gender. After a male holdout expressed anger participants began to doubt their original opinion more (i.e., the holdout influenced their opinion); whereas after a woman expressed anger, participants became more confident in their original opinion. Thus, men holdouts gained influence by expressing anger, but women were penalized for expressing anger. Women might be penalized because they are violating a stereotype by expressing anger, or because a historically disadvantaged group is expressing a dominant emotion (i.e., anger). We tested these two explanations by investigating whether the effect replicated for another historically underrepresented group for whom expressing anger would not violate, but instead confirm, a stereotype: Black men. We again found that men gained influence over the majority by expressing anger, but in contrast to the gender effect in Study 2, this effect did not depend on race of the holdout. After men holdouts expressed anger—regardless of whether they were Black or White—participants’ doubt in their original opinion decreased. Together, these studies reveal that expressing anger can greatly affect holdout jurors’ chances of influencing the majority—this effect depends, however, on the holdout’s social group. Although men were able to gain influence by expressing anger, women were penalized. Yet, not all historically disadvantaged groups are penalized—Black men were rewarded for expressing anger similarly to White men. Intellectual merit These studies demonstrated that expressing anger greatly affects holdouts’ chances of influencing the majority—but this effect depends on the holdout’s gender. Expressing anger created a gender gap in influence that did not exist when the holdouts expressed no emotion or fear. Men gained influence by expressing anger, whereas women were penalized. Yet, Black men were not penalized for expressing anger—instead rewarded similar to White men. Thus, we integrated and enriched three literatures by demonstrating a novel, complex interplay of stereotypes, emotion expression, and minority influence—relevant not only to legal decision making, but potentially other emotionally charged settings in which diverse groups must reach consensus. Broader impacts This research increased understanding of whether underrepresented jurors have the same opportunity to exert influence on jury verdicts, as do White men. This research contributes to socially relevant outcomes that the NSF values, such as full participation of women and underrepresented minorities and improved well-being of these individuals in society. First, this research increased our understanding about how, given the current social climate, women and racial minorities have differential abilities to exert influence over group decisions in emotionally charged settings. This finding is critical to underrepresented minorities’ participation and influence, not only on juries and therefore the justice system, but also in academic and STEM settings (e.g., classrooms, collaborator meetings, committees, workplace groups).

Agency
National Science Foundation (NSF)
Institute
Division of Social and Economic Sciences (SES)
Type
Standard Grant (Standard)
Application #
1123147
Program Officer
Marjorie Zatz
Project Start
Project End
Budget Start
2011-07-15
Budget End
2013-06-30
Support Year
Fiscal Year
2011
Total Cost
$12,878
Indirect Cost
Name
University of Illinois at Chicago
Department
Type
DUNS #
City
Chicago
State
IL
Country
United States
Zip Code
60612